
by Stan Purdum
In my recent article “The GPS in Your Head”, I mentioned that not every cyclist seems to have the “aptitude that makes stringing streets, roads and paths together to create bike routes easy.” Kerry Irons, one of the readers who commented on the article, confirmed that observation by telling of riding with a PhD physicist in a county where virtually every road was laid out on a grid.
“Roads were either north/south or east/west,” said Irons, “and many of them were numbered (e.g. 11 Mile Rd.) all referenced to a meridian and a base line. Roads were spaced one mile apart or at least in multiples of one mile.” Despite this seemingly intuitive layout, said Irons, his PhD friend “never knew where we were, never knew what direction we were travelling, and never knew which way to turn if somebody said something like ‘Let’s go north.’”
Irons added, “Whether nature or nurture, some folks just don’t ‘get it.’”
Another commenter, Charles, likewise attested to the fact that not everyone gets it. “I have found myself … being a default route finder/navigator for bike rides and other trips/excursions. Some of my riding friends have taken to calling me ‘Magellan’ although my ability/interest in finding my way around just seems to me to be an innate aptitude, and I often find myself amused that others are confused/lost when the route is trivially obvious to me.”
Irons also recommended a book on this subject, Inner Navigation, by Erik Jonsson, and I ordered a copy. While my inner navigation ability is pretty good, the topic interests me, and I wondered if Jonsson would elucidate why not everyone has that ability. I was somewhat surprised that he neither delved into that topic nor spent any time on how one might improve one’s internal route-finding skills. Nonetheless, he does an excellent job of describing what cognitive maps are, how they are developed, how they function, and how they can be fooled (misoriented).
Jonsson did give what seemed to me a clue as to why not everyone has good inner navigation skills. In describing how cognitive maps form, Jonsson said, “Our natural curiosity, the interest with which we look at new things, especially those that stand out as landmarks, is enough to create the cognitive map without any conscious effort.”
But what if someone doesn’t have that “natural curiosity,” at least about landmarks? Does the lack of such curiosity hinder the development of one’s cognitive map? I suspect so. My wife can read while in a moving vehicle (something that makes me nauseous when I try it), so when we are on long journeys and I am at the wheel, she often reads books or magazines. She’s smart and knows lots of stuff about things that don’t particularly interest me. But periodically on the trip, she may look up from what she’s been engrossed in and say, “Where are we?” I’m always able to answer because I’m doing the route plotting and watching the road signs, but if we’re on a route we’ve traveled before, I also know the answer because I’m always noticing — and apparently remembering — various unique buildings, geographic features, highway intersections, roadside vistas and other landmarks that help orient me.
My wife can find her way when she needs too, but it doesn’t come naturally to her, and she prefers to let me do when we’re together. Now, thanks to Jonsson, I suspect that’s because she doesn’t have the interest in visual landmarks that I do.
In my case, I know that interest was there from a young age. From the time I was 12 until I was 14, my family lived in Saratoga Springs, New York, and Saratoga Spa State Park was a couple miles from our house. The park had several attractions, but what drew me to it repeatedly was its natural environment — its forested grounds and hiking trails. I walked to the park as often as I could. I had a map of the park, the sort produced for visitors, and it showed its roads, picnic areas, buildings, attractions and the waterway that coursed through the grounds, but none of the park’s trails. So, over two summers, I spent many happy hours following every trail, figuring out my own links between them, and penciling them in on the map.
Here’s a bit more from Erik Jonsson:
“The cognitive map … is tailor made for us, showing only what we need to see. In contrast, a street map … shows mostly what we don’t need, and it takes quite a bit of practice in map reading to use it efficiently, to get past the wealth of useless information and find what one actually needs.”
“… if you have lived for a long time in a place and thus have a great deal of information stored in your spatial memory, the spatial system will automatically sort out just what you need at any given moment and present you with a cognitive map showing only where you are and the streets that will take you to your destination.”
“When we want to go to a faraway familiar place, we first see the direction to it in our cognitive map. We see it from where we are without any detail; it is just an awareness of direction and a distance. Later when we think about how to get there, we picture the route we have to follow. Usually we cannot visualize this route as one picture all the way from our starting point to our destination with sufficient detail to be useful; instead a cognitive route map is a series of pictures seen from points along the route and in the direction in which we are traveling. It is as if we, when we encoded the map of this route earlier, had a camera and snapped a picture every time something interesting appeared ahead. This means that a monotonous boring part is neglected on our map, whereas a stretch with many outstanding landmarks is well covered. … all the places along this route where we could possibly take the wrong road (like forks and intersections) are shown with enough detail to prevent mistakes.”
The book was published in 2002, before GPS units were as ubiquitous as they are today, and Jonsson mentions them only in passing.
Good comment on the “interest” issue. 6 years ago we moved to an area that was completely new to us. My wife and I are both “map people” and so purchased local paper maps and studied Google Maps to understand our new geography. The two counties where I do my riding have road numbering systems that allow you to calculate distances by knowing road numbers. Having lived here a couple of years, I knew the local roads better than many long-time local riders. Apparently, they just weren’t interested in knowing road names and distances, and so they started asking me for distances and directions.
I get around the Puget Sound area pretty well, but my usual riding buddy – a high school math teacher, which includes geometry – has no sense of direction. But perhaps more, not much sense of distance I think. And may not much sense of the days riding history. I can’t be sure of this, but it all seems consistent with the funny stuff that has happened in these past years. Those 3 attributes, plus a sense of the road system layout, town names, neighborhood names certainly helps.
But another curious thing that I have experienced several times is – I don’t know exactly where I am but suddenly feel a strong feeling of deja vu. The lay of the land plus the buildings or trees or whatever makes me think I have been there before. I used to keep silent about it because it was so bizarre. But it happened enough times where it proved out to be true that I now accept is as fact and trust it to get me where I want to go. It is uncanny. Has anyone else experienced this?
Seven years ago we relocated from Central Pennsylvania, which has fantastic country roads for road cycling. It also has many landmarks that help guide your way. We would begin riding a new route using a paper cue sheet but after riding it a number of times the cue sheet was no longer needed. You simply learned the turns by the landmarks, i,e,, bank building, small cemetery entrance, yellow house, etc, When we began riding in metro Phoenix we quickly learned landmarks would not be your guide, The area is laid out on a one mile grid and every intersection looks the same from the color of everything( tan or brown of course since we live in the desert) to the stores in the shopping centers ( the same chain stores in every shopping center as there are few if any “Mom and Pop” stores), Even though we ride a finite number of different routes, we rely on our Garmins to give us the turns for rides going 40-75 miles. Having the Garmins give you the turns also frees up your attention so that you can pay more attention to your safety in traffic. Road cycling here is very different from riding in less urban areas. Nowadays, with GPS for bicycles, an innate sense of direction is not all that important,